Sonja Graafstal en Carine Heijligers

602 LITERATUUR Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner, & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 793–828). Wiley. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2007). The bioecological model of human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 793–828). Wiley. https://doi. org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0114 Bruin, E. de (2020, 29 januari). Waarom klinkt dat zo grappig? NRC. https://www.nrc.nl/ nieuws/2020/01/29/waarom-deze-woorden-grappig-klinken-dat-is-onderzocht-a3988570 Brzović, Z. (2022). Natural kinds. Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. https://iep.utm.edu/natkind/ Buck, J. B. (1938). Synchronous rhythmic flashing of fireflies. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 13(3), 301–314. http://doi.org/10.1086/394562 Bullowa, M. (1979). Before speech. The beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge University Press. Butler, E.A. (2011). Temporal interpersonal emotion systems: The “TIES” that form relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 367–393. http://doi. org/10.1177/1088868311411164 Butler, E.A. (2015). Interpersonal affect dynamics: It takes two (and time) to tango. Emotion Review, 7(4), 336-341. http://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915590622 Buytendijk, F. J. J. (1948). Algemene theorie over de menselijke houding en beweging. Spectrum. Carr, A. (2006). The handbook of child and adolescent clinical psychology. A contextual approach (2nd ed.). Routledge. Cator, L. J., Arthur, B. J., Harrington, L. C., & Hoy, R. R. (2009). Harmonic convergence in the love songs of the dengue vector mosquito. Science, 323(5917), 1077–1079. https://doi. org/10.1126/science.1166541 Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910. https://doi. org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893 Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70(7), 741–756. https:// doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba Cirelli L. K., Wan S. J., & Trainor L. J. (2014). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369: 20130400. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0400 Clauwaert, S. (2015a). Descriptieve bewijskracht. Omschrijving van de methode contactgericht spelen en leren. Stichting Horison. https://www.horison.nl/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/ omschrijvingCSLstap1.pdf Clauwaert, S. (2015b). Interventietheorie. Theoretische onderbouwing van de methode contactgericht spelen en leren. Stichting Horison. https://www.horison.nl/wp-content/ uploads/2016/02/theoretischeonderbouwingCSLstap2.pdf Cohan, S. L., Price, J. M., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Suffering in silence: Why a developmental psychopathology perspective on selective mutism is needed. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(4), 341–355. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200608000-00011 Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1987). Mother–infant face-to-face interaction: The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 68–77. https://doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.68 Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner’s behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 386–392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.3.386

RkJQdWJsaXNoZXIy MTk4NDMw