Sonja Graafstal en Carine Heijligers

610 LITERATUUR Mason, G. J., & Turner, M. A. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the development and control of stereotypies. In P. P. G. Bateson, P. H. Klopfer, & N.S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. Behavior and evolution (pp. 53–85). Plenum Press. May, R. M. (1976). Simple mathematical models with very complicated dynamics. Nature 261, 459–467. https://doi.org/10.1038/261459a0 McConkey, R., Mulvany, F., & Barron, S. (2006). Adult persons with intellectual disabilities on the island of Ireland. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 50(3), 227–236. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00777.x McInnis, E. E., Hills, A., & Chapman, M. J. (2012). Eligibility for statutory learning disability services in the north‐west of England. Right or luxury? Findings from a pilot study. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(3), 177–186. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-3156.2011.00691.x McKeon, R. (1947). Introduction to Aristotle. Random House. McKinney, C., & Renk, K. (2007). Emerging research and theory in the etiology of oppositional defiant disorder: Current concerns and future directions. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 3(3), 349–371. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0100811 Meer, M. van der (2004). Ik verbind u door. Atlas Contact. Meltzoff, A. N. (1990). Towards a developmental cognitive science: the implications of crossmodal matching and imitation for the development of representation and memory in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608(1), 1–31. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48889.x Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W. (1979) Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/282403a0 Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.198.4312.75 Mercer, J. (2017). Examining DIR/Floortime™ as a treatment for children with autism spectrum disorders: a review of research and theory. Research on Social Work Practice, 27(5), 625–635. http://doi.org/10.1177/1049731515583062 Merton, R. K. (1968). The Matthew effect in science. Science 159(3810), 56–63. http://doi. org/10.1126/science.159.3810.56 Mitchell, M., & Newman, M. (2002). Complex systems theory and evolution. http://www-personal. umich.edu/~mejn/papers/cs.ps Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009 Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2(4), 201–218. http://dx.doi. org/10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1 Molenaar, P. C. M. (2008). On the implications of the classical ergodic theorems: Analysis of developmental processes has to focus on intra-individual variation. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(1), 60–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.20262 Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 112–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.14678721.2009.01619.x Montague, D. P. F., & Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: A new look at infants’ perception of emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 826–838. https:// doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.826 Moonen, X. (2015). Is inclusie van mensen met een verstandelijke beperking vanzelfsprekend? [Inaugurele rede]. Zuyd. https://www.zuyd.nl/binaries/content/assets/zuyd/onderzoek/ inaugurele-redes/inclusie-mensen-verstandelijke-beperking---lectorale-rede-xaviermoonen.pdf

RkJQdWJsaXNoZXIy MTk4NDMw